Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 March 2007 Documentation of Infanticide and Cannibalism in Bald Eagles
A. Catherine Markham, Bryan D. Watts
Author Affiliations +

Non-kin infanticide, the killing of dependent young by unrelated conspecifics, occurs in a wide array of taxonomic groups including mammals, insects, fish, and birds (Hrdy 1979, Hrdy and Hausfater 1984). Several hypotheses have been proposed to explain this behavior including the removal of potential competitors to gain access to limited resources such as nesting territories and food (Hrdy 1979).

In many social contexts, the adaptive advantages of cannibalism, the consumption of conspecifics, are closely linked with infanticide (Mock 1984). For example, cannibalism may provide nourishment, lower the reproductive success of competitors, and decrease intraspecific competition for resources by lowering population density (Stanback and Koenig 1992). As the fitness benefits of infanticide and cannibalism are not mutually exclusive, Stanback and Koenig (1992) note that it is “difficult to distinguish between the following scenarios: (a) selection that favors cannibalism, for which infanticide is often a necessary prerequisite, and (b) selection that favors infanticide, for which cannibalism is a subsequent option.”

Among birds, infanticide encompasses a variety of species and social contexts (Mock 1984). Several studies have reported infanticide by adult males (e.g., Crook and Shields 1985, Freed 1986), which often has been interpreted as a means of obtaining breeding opportunities under conditions of high competition for mates or nest sites. Some accounts document cannibalism following the killing of young by unrelated adults (e.g., Balda and Bateman 1976, Village 1983). However, among raptors, direct observations of infanticide are relatively rare (but see Wiemeyer 1981, Bortolotti et al. 1991, Webster et al. 1999) and we found no reports of cannibalism linked with infanticide for Bald Eagles (Haliaeetus leucocephalus). This scarcity of confirmed occurrences may be due, at least in part, to the combined relative infrequency of such events and the difficulty in continuously monitoring nest activities. Here we describe a case of infanticide and cannibalism in the Bald Eagle.

Study Area and Methods

We video-monitored a Bald Eagle nest on a tributary of the James River in Virginia in 2002 as part of a larger study on the diet and provisioning patterns of nesting Bald Eagles in the lower Chesapeake Bay. A small, bullet security camera (Speco Technologies, CVC-320WP, Amityville, NY U.S.A.) was mounted directly above the nest and wired to power (a deep-cycle 12-V marine battery) and recording (a time-lapse videocassette recorder, HS-9596U, Mitsubishi Digital Electronics America, Inc., Irvine, CA U.S.A.) equipment positioned 250 m from the base of the nest tree. Additional details of the camera system components are provided in Markham (2004).

We installed the camera system in January, prior to egg laying, and regularly monitored nest activities from 20 February–11 April 2002. We recorded nest activity during the incubation phase (26 d) and nestling phase before (1 d) and after (13 d) mate loss. We positioned the camera so that the entire surface of the nest was in view and made no attempts at additional observations other than those recorded on videotape. Recordings of nest activity typically included all daylight hours (sunrise to sunset).

Because the nest had multiple young, we used the date of first egg laying and first chick hatching to classify nesting phase. During tape review, we identified individual adult birds by unique plumage characteristics and differences in body size consistent with sexual dimorphism in the species (Bortolotti 1984). We also used relative body size to identify nestlings. Bald Eagle chicks hatch asynchronously (Gerrard and Bortolotti 1988); thus, nestlings shortly after hatch date are often easy to distinguish on the basis of relative size.

Results

At the start of video-monitoring, we observed both breeding adults contributing to nest maintenance. Recordings on 20 February marked our initial observation of the first egg and the laying of the second egg. By 24 February, the three-egg clutch was complete. The first chick hatched on 26 March after a minimum 34 d incubation. The second and third chicks hatched 3 and 4 d later, respectively. Throughout this period, we consistently observed both adults sharing incubation and brooding responsibilities.

Beginning 30 March (nestling ages 1–4 d), the resident adult male was no longer observed on the nest. We believe this mate died; an adult male was treated and released by a state biologist within 1 km of the nest site earlier in the nesting season (exact date not recorded by wildlife officials). The bird had suffered injuries after a fishhook became imbedded in its wing. Though the damage was assessed as minor and the bird was released immediately, concurrence of this event with the disappearance of the resident male suggests that the injuries may have been fatal.

On 3 April, the youngest of three chicks (4 d) was lethargic and did not exhibit food begging behavior, while its siblings were active and readily begged for and received food. The youngest chick was brooded by the female overnight, but was motionless and presumed dead when first visible the following morning. The adult attempted to preferentially brood the dead chick on two occasions, leaving both older siblings exposed. On 4 April at 1317 H, the first instance of cannibalism was observed when the adult picked up the dead chick's body and began feeding it to the other nestlings.

Two additional incidences of cannibalism occurred at this nest, this time in conjunction with infanticide, over a 2-d period during which another Bald Eagle was repeatedly observed on the nest. Based on size dimorphism in Bald Eagles (Bortolotti 1984), we identified this bird as male, and age-specific plumage patterns (McCollough 1989) indicated it was 4 yr old. On the first day, 9 April, we observed the female delivering prey items to the nest, brooding, and provisioning the chicks. At 1552 H, following a feeding bout, the female left the nest and the 4-yr-old male arrived within 1 min. The male approached the chicks and grasped the larger (older; 11 d) of the two in its talons, moved it to the nest edge, and pinned it down while killing and eating it. At 1610 H, the male left the larger chick's carcass and repeated the same behavior of grasping and pinning the second (sole remaining) chick (10 d), although not killing it. The adult female arrived on the nest at 1611 H and immediately flushed the 4-yr-old male from the camera view.

On the second day, 10 April, we witnessed a similar pattern of intrusion followed by infanticide and cannibalism. The 4-yr-old male entered the nest twice when the female was absent from view. The female returned to the nest within 1 min on both occasions, immediately flushing the male. Intermittently, the female attempted to brood and feed the remaining live chick. At 1224 H, after the absence of the female for over 90 min, the male landed on the nest and began picking at prey remains. After alternating between a prey item and the last living nestling, the adult pinned the struggling chick down with its talons and began ripping the nestling apart with its beak. At 1249 H, 1 min after the 4-yr-old male finished consuming the nestling, the female joined the 4-yr-old male on the nest. After calling at the 4-yr-old male for 40 sec, the female stepped toward him and flushed him from view. Later in the day, the female delivered and consumed one prey item on the nest surface. The 4-yr-old male returned to the nest periodically, occasionally bringing nesting material (sticks) and arranging the nest lining. On two subsequent occasions, the female chased the 4-yr-old male off the nest.

The following day, 11 April, both the resident female and the 4-yr-old male were observed on the nest again. The female again chased the 4-yr-old male off the nest, but it appeared that both eagles had been roosting near the nest (the 4-yr-old male landed on the nest surface at 0604 H, before sunrise, followed by the female within 20 sec). Following this encounter, the 4-yr-old male was not observed on the nest again in the remaining hours of observation. The female delivered and consumed one prey item on the nest surface later that day.

Discussion

Agonistic interactions among adult and subadult Bald Eagles are well-documented, yet observations of direct conflict between immatures and nestlings are considerably less common. While video-monitoring other Bald Eagle nests in the Chesapeake Bay region during the 2002–03 breeding seasons, we observed several instances in which immature eagles (between the ages of 2 yr and 4 yr old) landed on occupied nests while the breeding pair was out of sight (away from the nest surface). In none of these instances did the immature eagle directly interact with the nestlings. However, P. Nye (pers. comm.) documented a case in New York in which a dead 4-wk-old eaglet was found below a nest several days after a 2-yr-old Bald Eagle was seen repeatedly landing on the occupied nest (in each observed instance, the subadult was driven off by the resident breeders); necropsy results revealed talon punctures on the nestling. Such evidence, along with relatively more common findings of intraspecific predation of chicks by adults in other raptor species (e.g., Rosenfield and Papp 1988, Bortolotti et al. 1991), suggests the events described here may have been a simple case of nest predation by the 4-yr-old male. We found no previous accounts of direct observations describing this behavior in Bald Eagles in the literature available to us.

Given the loss of the original adult male and subsequent appearance of a 4-yr-old male, another potential explanation for the observed account of infanticide is that these events occurred in the context of mate replacement, following the original male's disappearance from the nest site. Mate replacement has been previously documented in Bald Eagles both within (Kozie 1986, Grubb et al. 1988, Jenkins and Jackman 1993, Anthony et al. 1994, Kennedy and McTaggart-Cowan 1998) and between (Herrick 1932) breeding seasons. Though we know of no direct accounts of infanticide following mate replacement in Bald Eagles, some observations strongly suggest this occurs. P. Nye (pers. comm.) observed a case in which a breeding female was driven from her nest containing a single 1-wk-old chick by an intruder female. The original female was repeatedly harassed by the intruder and eventually moved to a new nest site, while the intruder remained on the territory with the original male; the single chick of the original breeding pair was not seen following the replacement and observers presumed it was killed by the intruder female (P. Nye pers. comm.).

Among avian species, the act of unrelated adults killing young has typically been interpreted as a means through which the adult can secure mating opportunities sooner than would be possible if the offspring continued to be reared (Crook and Shields 1985, Moller 1988). However, of the theoretical models described by Rohwer (1986) for mate replacement during a nesting season, the natural history characteristics of the Bald Eagle (i.e., unlikely renesting within the same season and increased breeding success with experience) suggest that adoption of existing clutches may be more adaptive to securing a position as a replacement adult than infanticide. In observations of mate replacement during the breeding season, Grubb et al. (1988) observed the replacement male assisting the female in parental duties. Adoption of nestlings has also been observed in fostering experiments, suggesting that Bald Eagles will accept and raise offspring other than their own. For example, Postupalsky (1975) reports on the successful introduction of 9-wk-old nestlings into eagle nests where the breeding pair was intact. Similarly, over a 5-yr study period, 8 nestlings between the ages of 2.5 wk and 4 wk were successfully fostered to mated pairs in exchange for either eggs or chicks (P. Nye pers. comm.). However, in observations made by Jenkins and Jackman (1993), a replacement female remained indifferent to eggs already present on the nest despite the attempt of the male parent to continue incubation.

Why different behavioral strategies were followed in each of these situations is not clear. The case of indifference may be explained as a gender distinction in adaptive strategies, perhaps varying with the adult's age and mating experience, as sexually-selected replacement behavior has been shown to occur in other avian species (e.g., Crook and Shields 1985, Freed 1986, Robertson and Stutchbury 1988). Among the accounts involving male replacement, the variable responses may have been due to differences in nesting stage at the time of replacement. The account by Grubb et al. (1988) occurred on the threshold of hatching, whereas the young in our observations were both killed when less than 2 wk old. Finally, the age of the replacement adult may also be a contributing factor. Anthony et al. (1994) reported that Bald Eagle nests where one member of the breeding pair had near-adult plumage, defined as having a partially white tail and horizontal line of dark feathers in the eye region, were rarely successful. Further, they concluded that nest failure in an observed instance of mate replacement was specifically attributed to the youth and inexperience of the replacement. Whether or not the age of the nestlings or the replacement adult were a significant factor in the actions we observed is not clear.

Unfortunately, no conclusive evidence is available on whether or not the 4-yr-old male later formed a pair bond with the resident female. A breeding attempt was documented at the nest the following year during aerial surveys (B. Watts and M. Byrd pers. comm.). However, identification of individuals in the breeding pair was not possible because the unique plumage characteristics used to distinguish the birds in 2002 would have been lost with annual feather molt. Thus, caution is needed in interpreting these accounts in the context of mate replacement as opposed to simple conspecific predation by a subadult.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Hoffler Creek Nature Preserve for allowing access to the study site. We also thank K. Cline and B. Paxton for giving graciously of their time in the field. D. Schwab with the Virginia Department of Game and Inland Fisheries provided information about the injured adult male Bald Eagle. H. Austin, J. Bednarz, M. Byrd, P. Heideman, F. Isaacs, and P. Nye offered helpful comments on this manuscript. This study was financially supported by the Center for Conservation Biology and grants from the College of William and Mary, the Mary and Daniel Loughran Foundation, the Northern Neck Audubon Society, the Virginia Society of Ornithology, and the Williamsburg Bird Club.

Literature Cited

1.

R. G. Anthony, R. W. Frenzel, F. B. Isaacs, and M. G. Garrett . 1994. Probable causes of nesting failures in Oregon's Bald Eagle population. Wildl. Soc. Bull 22:576–582. Google Scholar

2.

R. P. Balda and G. C. Bateman . 1976. Cannibalism in the Piñon Jay. Condor 78:562–564. Google Scholar

3.

G. R. Bortolotti 1984. Sexual size dimorphism and age-related size variation in Bald Eagles. J. Wildl. Manage 48:72–81. Google Scholar

4.

G. R. Bortolotti, K. L. Wiebe, and W. M. Iko . 1991. Cannibalism of nestling American Kestrels by their parents and siblings. Can. J. Zool 69:1447–1453. Google Scholar

5.

J. R. Crook and W. M. Shields . 1985. Sexually selected infanticide by adult male Barn Swallows. Anim. Behav 33:754–761. Google Scholar

6.

L. A. Freed 1986. Territory takeover and sexually selected infanticide in Tropical House Wrens. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 19:197–206. Google Scholar

7.

J. M. Gerrard and G. R. Bortolotti . 1988. The Bald Eagle: haunts and habits of a wilderness monarch. Smithsonian Inst. Press. Washington, DC U.S.A. Google Scholar

8.

T. G. Grubb, L. A. Forbis, M. McWhorter, and D. R. Sherman . 1988. Adaptive perch selection as a mechanism of adoption by a replacement Bald Eagle. Wilson Bull 100:302–305. Google Scholar

9.

F. H. Herrick 1932. Daily life of the American eagle: early phase. Auk 49:307–323. Google Scholar

10.

S. B. Hrdy 1979. Infanticide among animals: a review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethol. Sociobiol 1:13–40. Google Scholar

11.

S. B. Hrdy and G. Hausfater . 1984. Comparative and evolutionary perspectives on infanticide: introduction and overview. xiii–xxxv. In G. Hausfater and S. B. Hrdy , editors. eds. Infanticide: comparative and evolutionary perspectives. Aldine Press. New York, NY U.S.A. Google Scholar

12.

J. M. Jenkins and R. E. Jackman . 1993. Mate and nest site fidelity in a resident population of Bald Eagles. Condor 95:1053–1056. Google Scholar

13.

E. Kennedy and I. McTaggart-Cowan . 1998. Sixteen years with a Bald Eagle's, Haliaeetus leucocephalus, nest. Can. Field-Nat 112:704–706. Google Scholar

14.

K. D. Kozie 1986. Breeding and feeding ecology of Bald Eagles in the Apostle Islands National Lakeshore. M.S. thesis,. Univ. of Wisconsin. Stevens Point, WI U.S.A. Google Scholar

15.

A. C. Markham 2004. The influence of salinity on diet composition, provisioning patterns, and nestling growth in Bald Eagles in the lower Chesapeake Bay. M.A. thesis,. College of William and Mary. Williamsburg, VA U.S.A. Google Scholar

16.

M. A. McCollough 1989. Molting sequence and aging of Bald Eagles. Wilson. Bull 101:1–10. Google Scholar

17.

D. W. Mock 1984. Infanticide, siblicide, and avian nestling mortality. 3–30. In G. Hausfater and S. B. Hrdy , editors. eds. Infanticide: comparative and evolutionary perspectives. Aldine Press. New York, NY U.S.A. Google Scholar

18.

A. P. Moller 1988. Infanticidal and anti-infanticidal strategies in the swallow Hirundi rustica. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 22:365–371. Google Scholar

19.

S. Postupalsky 1975. Adoption of nestlings by breeding Bald Eagles. Raptor Res 9:18–20. Google Scholar

20.

R. J. Robertson and B. J. Stutchbury . 1988. Experimental evidence for sexually selected infanticide in Tree Swallows. Anim. Behav 36:749–753. Google Scholar

21.

S. Rohwer 1986. Selection for adoption versus infanticide by replacement “mates” in birds. 353–395. In R. F. Johnston , editor. ed. Current ornithology. Plenum Press. New York, NY U.S.A. Google Scholar

22.

R. N. Rosenfield and J. M. Papp . 1988. Subadult intrusion and probable infanticide at a Cooper's Hawk nest. Wilson Bull 100:506–507. Google Scholar

23.

M. T. Stanback and W. D. Koenig . 1992. Cannibalism in birds. 277–298. In M. A. Elgar and B. J. Crespi , editors. eds. Cannibalism: ecology and evolution among diverse taxa. Oxford Univ. Press. Oxford, U.K. Google Scholar

24.

A. Village 1983. The role of nest-site availability and territorial behaviour in limiting the breeding density of Kestrels. J. Anim. Ecol 52:635–645. Google Scholar

25.

A. Webster, R. Cooke, G. Jameson, and R. Wallis . 1999. Diet, roosts and breeding of Powerful Owls Ninox strenua in a disturbed, urban environment: a case for cannibalism? Or a case of infanticide? Emu 99:80–83. Google Scholar

26.

S. N. Wiemeyer 1981. Captive propagation of Bald Eagles at Patuxent Wildlife Research Center and introductions into the wild, 1976–80. Raptor. Res 15:68–82. Google Scholar
A. Catherine Markham and Bryan D. Watts "Documentation of Infanticide and Cannibalism in Bald Eagles," Journal of Raptor Research 41(1), 41-44, (1 March 2007). https://doi.org/10.3356/0892-1016(2007)41[41:DOIACI]2.0.CO;2
Received: 19 September 2005; Accepted: 1 December 2006; Published: 1 March 2007
KEYWORDS
Bald Eagle
cannibalism
Haliaeetus leucocephalus
infanticide
mate loss
Back to Top